Posted by: Gary Ernest Davis on: April 21, 2011
Mathematics
In a previous post I described Hermann Ebbinghaus’s experiments on rote memorization, and his finding that memorization of nonsense syllables decays exponentially.
What we teachers of mathematics want is for students to recall, at least for the very next class, what came just before.
Ideally, education should set up long term – very long term – memories so that an educated person can recall basic facts and procedures many, many years after learning them.
A critical aspect of memory formation is focus of attention, and this is an important issue I will return to later.
Here I want to focus on an aspect of memory that we use as we carry out various tasks: working memory.
Memory research boomed in the late 1900’s, so much so that in 2000 a monumental handbook of memory research was published by Oxford University Press.
One of the best, most readable, introductions to the field of memory research, by two of the main contributors, is “Memory. From Mind to Molecules.” by Larry Squire & Eric Kandel.
Using Wikipedia’s open source book creator I have pulled together a collection of Wikipedia articles related to this post: Working_Memory_WikiBook
Dissatisfied with inconsistent theories of short-term memory, Allen Baddeley & Graham Hitch introduced in 1974 a multi-component theory of working memory.
Roughly speaking, we can think of working memory as that aspect of memory we bring to bear on holding relevant information in the forefront of our minds as carry out more or less complex tasks.
Baddeley and Hitch proposed that working memory consists in broad outline of a central executive that coordinates information, and two “slave” systems, the phonological loop that stores and updates information about sounds, and a visuo-spatial sketch pad that stores and updates visual information.
Neurological studies have confirmed the Baddeley-Hitch model in broad outline (Oxford Handbook of Memory, pp. 77-92)
The need for teachers of mathematics to know something about the functioning of working memory is clear: students must use working memory in order to solve mathematical problems.
The need for mathematics teachers to know details about working memory would be less were it not for the empirical fact that working memory is very limited in its capacity, and training in the use of working memory is generally not effective in increasing its capacity.
What this means in practice is that mathematics teachers need to be very aware of the cognitive load of a mathematical problem – the stress the problem is likely to place on individual student’s working memory.
The explicit concept of cognitive load originated with George Miller and was widely elucidated in problem solving by John Sweller.
In an earlier post I wrote about reducing cognitive load in algebra, and how John Sweller’s ideas, which I misunderstood for a long time, are very helpful in reducing cognitive load in mathematical problem solving.
I also wrote in an earlier post how the wording of a problem can significantly increase the cognitive load for a student.
The example used there was the following:
Sandy’s family does its laundry at a coin-operated laundromat. It costs $1.25 per load to use the washing machines and 25¢ per load to use the dryers for 10 minutes. Sandy’s family has 5 loads of laundry to do and each load will need to be in a dryer for 30 minutes. Which expression will give Sandy’s family the total cost of doing these loads of laundry?
A. ($1.25 + $0.25) × 3 × 5
B. [$1.25 + (3 × $0.25)] × 5
C. [(3 × $1.25) + $0.25] × 5
D. 3 × ($1.25 + $0.25) × 5
From the 2002 Washington Assessment of Student Learning (WASL) for Mathematics.
About half (49.9%) of more than 70,000 students who took the test were unable to manage the cognitive demands of this problem to select the correct answer. (Choice B)
About one-third (33.9%) of the students linked the information within the second sentence (Choices A and D) instead of linking information from the second sentence with information from the third sentence.
Students’ lack of prior experience with laundromats could be expected to impact their success rate.
What is really being tested by this problem?
Use of the pronoun it in the sentence, “It costs $1.25 per load to use the washing machines and 25¢ per load to use the dryers for 10 min,â€
Compounding & cognitive demands; “and 25¢ per load,†links the “25¢ per load†with the washing machine load until the end of the sentence -prior experience with laundromats signals the reader to interpret the sentence differently.
Spanish speakers, for example, may interpret the words to do as make.
Knowledge assumptions: a student may be familiar with wash clothes in English but not with the word laundry, and students whose families do laundry at home may not understand the word laundromat.
Load is usually first learned as a verb and when second-language learners read the word expression, they may, for example, think about what someone’s face looks like.
For many ESL students, English first names are not easily recognized; “What is a Sandy?â€
Empirically, some problems seem to increase cognitive load for many students, irrespective of how they are worded. The problem at the very beginning of this post is such an example.
Why this should be so is not at all clear.
Leave a Reply